Wednesday, February 17, 2016

Post-Doc with Tecumseh Fitch in Vienna Department of Cognitive Biology

A post-doctoral research position is available in the Department of Cognitive Biology at the School of Life Sciences, University of Vienna, in the laboratory of Prof W Tecumseh Fitch. (duration up to five years). The main topic(s) of the position are flexible and will depend upon the skills and interests of the chosen applicant. In general, comparative research in bioacoustics, biomusicology and animal cognition will be favored. For example, ongoing research in the Fitch lab concerns the biology and evolution of language and music and the cognitive capacities underlying pattern perception in speech, music and complex visual patterns; we work with both humans and non-human animals including birds (ravens, parrots) and primates (marmosets, humans). For these topics, a strong background in the design and analysis of experiments is key, and experience with nonhuman animals a strong plus. A somewhat independent research topic focuses on vertebrate bioacoustics, particularly vocal production in birds and mammals. For these topics, a strong background in speech science, bioacoustics and/or the physcs and physiology of vocal production would be central. Qualified applicants could also combine research in both sets of topics.

You will be joining a very active and well-funded group of scientists in the Department of Cognitive Biology at the University of Vienna (the core professors are Thomas Bugnyar, Tecumseh Fitch and Leonida Fusani). We have strong links with the laboratory of Prof. Ludwig Huber at the University of Veterinary Medicine, Vienna. The general focus of our department is comparative analysis, with a wide range of vertebrate species, and all topics relevant to the evolution of the mind. Species currently available for study on site include ravens, pigeons, keas, budgerigars, dogs, and common marmosets; we have new an fully-equipped animal facilities in Vienna, and at our new field station south of Vienna; other species can be studied via collaborations elsewhere (e.g. chimpanzees, wolves). We have a fully-equipped animal care facility (see below), human testing labs including a large anechoic chamber, an EyeLink 1000 eye tracker and Biopack physiological measurement system.

Full details here.

Tuesday, February 16, 2016

What’s the right hemisphere doing? [Guest Post by William Matchin]


Guest post by William Matchin:

What's the right hemisphere doing?

This is a question that has been bothering me for about two years, emerging from the results of an fMRI experiment that I performed in the twilight of graduate school, and recently butting into my consciousness again after Greg and I finally published the paper. The paper is called “‘Syntactic perturbation’ activates the right IFG, but not Broca’s area or the ATL”, recently published in Frontiers in Psychology as part of a special topic on “Components of the Language-Ready Brain” edited by Cedric Boeckx and Antonio Benítez-Burraco. The title is a bit of a mouthful, I know. What we showed is that when you’re producing a sentence, if you’re forced to change the structure of the sentence-mid utterance, the right IFG (among other areas) lights up like a Christmas tree.



I think it’s a good paper, but what’s important about it are not the specific results, but rather the larger question that the results point toward. Hemispheric asymmetries were all the rage in the 60s and 70s, but have died out in the last couple of decades. However, just as Mad Men brought back 60s fashion and interior decor, I’d like to bring back the hemisphere question.

So let me ask again. What’s the right hemisphere doing?

Any ideas?

I think there are some reasons to suggest that this question is actually important (you know, for science) rather than for the purposes of explaining that line in the table of significant activations in your fMRI experiment. Let me lay out some of the reasons why there is a real question here. What I really want to do is find out if other people have also thought about this, and what the contents of those thoughts are.

Reason #1: the right hemisphere activates a lot for sentence processing

Does anyone ever look at images like this (taken from Tyler et al., 2011) and wonder what that right hemisphere activation reflects?



Given the received wisdom of left hemisphere language dominance, these are pretty unexpected results. In fact, many of these studies have used manipulations that, when getting activation in left IFG, supposedly justify this region’s involvement in syntax – the core component of language that is putatively specific to it (Hauser et al., 2002; Bolhuis et al., 2014). Take syntactic violations. Here’s an exercise – find a study of syntactic violations that doesn’t activate right IFG.  You won’t find any – they all show increased activation in the right IFG. If increased activation for syntactic violations justifies the left IFG’s role in core syntax, then you’d be justifying the right IFG’s role in core syntax. I don’t think either conclusion is warranted, but the results need to be explained.

The right IFG often activates for ambiguity (Zempleni et al., 2007; Tyler et al., 2011), garden-path sentences (Chan et al., 2012), non-canonical structures (Fiebach et al., 2005; Bornkessel-Schlesewsky et al., 2010), and for switching sentence constructions (Matchin & Hickok, 2016).

I don’t think these results can be explained away very easily.

Reason # 2 – language dominance develops switches to the right hemisphere if the left is broken

In children, if you have a stroke in the left hemisphere, you surprisingly turn out to be pretty much fine with respect to language. Why? Because everything that the left hemisphere would have done switches to the right (Tivarus et al., 2012; Basser et al., 1962).

I think this points out pretty clearly that whatever the left hemisphere does in language, the right hemisphere can also do. This suggests that the left hemisphere acquires its affinity for language in some way that also allows language-dominance to emerge in the right hemisphere, such as similar connectivity. Doesn’t this suggest some interesting clues about how language grows in the brain? And doesn’t this also suggest some natural division in normal, right-handed adults, whereby the left and right hemisphere play some a symmetrical role with respect to sentence processing, operating over similar kinds of information but in different ways?

Reason #3 – you can do basic syntax without the left hemisphere

All through grad school Greg beat me over the head (but in a loving way) with the fact that Broca’s area doesn’t do syntax – patients with agrammatism, and presumably damage to Broca’s area, can do complex acceptability judgments without much problem (Linebarger et al., 1983; Wulfeck & Bates, 1991). Acceptability judgments are what syntacticians have used for decades to develop syntactic theory. Making correct judgments can often be hard for the syntacticians – the fact that patients with severe language disorder can make them is telling us a lot.

Interestingly, the right hemisphere of split-brain patients can also make acceptability judgments (Baynes & Gazzaniga, 1988). I think this means that the right hemisphere much have some kind of syntactic magic.

Reason # 4 – damage to the right hemisphere can impair sentence processing

There is less data for this one. There is some data, though – Caplan et al., 1996 show that damage to the right hemisphere can result in sentence comprehension problems, particularly for syntactically complex structures.

Other lines of work suggest that patients with right hemisphere damage have problems suppressing context-inappropriate interpretations (Tompkins et al., 2001), particularly important for getting the punchlines of jokes or understanding metaphors.

Reason # 5 – the right IFG is really important for inhibition in motor control

If you believe that there are useful analogies to be drawn between language and other domains, like vision or motor control, then we ought to pay attention to what these people are saying about the right hemisphere. Adam Aron has been claiming, with very good evidence, that the right IFG is the key region involved in “stopping” (Aron et al., 2014).

That is, you’re about to reach for that cookie, you spot your mother out of the corner of your eye, and you as sure as hell do not reach for the cookie. That’s stopping – the inhibition of a planned motor behavior.

In our Frontiers perturbation paper we pointed out the similarity of our activations to the stopping literature. Without the fear of the lash of reviewer B, I can speculate more freely here. My hypothesis is that in sentence processing you generate predictive templates for syntax and semantics in the same way that you generate motor plans. This has close similarities to work by Tom Bever and colleagues (Fodor et al., 1974). I think that generating these templates relies on the left IFG. If you need to cancel or change those schemata, you employ a stopping mechanism. This relies on the right IFG. Hence damage to the right hemisphere results in deficits for particular syntactic constructions or for obtaining non-literal interpretations of sentences – if you can’t suppress your original prediction, you’ll miss the joke or be unable to revise the predicted structure.

More broadly, both hemispheres have significant access to the basic syntactic mechanisms of language. In development, the left develops into a “go” function, and the right develops into a “stop” function. These complementary mechanisms are not set in stone, though – this allows for the right hemisphere to take on a “stop” function if the left is damaged, or for left-handers to have more even distribution of “stop” and “go” juice across the two hemispheres.

This makes some predictions – patients with right hemisphere damage should have some serious problems with things like garden-path sentences. More generally, subtle clinical testing should reveal a wide array of syntactic and semantic problems when inhibition is required.

What do you think? I am curious to hear what everyone has thought about the right hemisphere and its role in language broadly. Is it doing something interesting? Or should we just ignore these right hemisphere activations entirely?

References

Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., & Poldrack, R. A. (2014). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends in cognitive sciences, 18(4), 177-185.

Basser, L. S. (1962). Hemiplegia of early onset and the faculty of speech with special reference to the effects of hemispherectomy. Brain85(3), 427-460.

Bates, E., Wulfeck, B., & MacWhinney, B. (1991). Cross-linguistic research in aphasia: An overview. Brain and language41(2), 123-148.

Baynes, K., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1988). Right hemisphere language: Insights into normal language mechanisms. Language, communication, and the brain, 117-126.

Bolhuis, J. J., Tattersall, I., Chomsky, N., & Berwick, R. C. (2014). How could language have evolved?. PLoS Biol12(8), e1001934.

Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., Grewe, T., & Schlesewsky, M. (2012). Prominence vs. aboutness in sequencing: A functional distinction within the left inferior frontal gyrus. Brain and language120(2), 96-107.

Caplan, D., Hildebrandt, N., & Makris, N. (1996). Location of lesions in stroke patients with 679 deficits in syntactic processing in sentence comprehension. Brain, 119(3), 933-950.

Chan, Y. C., Chou, T. L., Chen, H. C., & Liang, K. C. (2012). Segregating the comprehension 681 and elaboration processing of verbal jokes: an fMRI study. NeuroImage, 61(4), 899-906.

Fiebach, C. J., Schlesewsky, M., Lohmann, G., Von Cramon, D. Y., & Friederici, A. D. (2005). Revisiting the role of Broca's area in sentence processing: syntactic integration versus syntactic working memory. Human brain mapping24(2), 79-91.

Fodor, J., Bever, T., & Garrett, M. (1974). The psychology of language: An introduction to psycholinguistics and generative grammar. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science298(5598), 1569-1579.

Linebarger, M. C., Schwartz, M. F., & Saffran, E. M. (1983). Sensitivity to grammatical structure in so-called agrammatic aphasics. Cognition13(3), 361-392.

Matchin, W. & Hickok, G. (2016, in press). ‘Syntactic perturbation’ during production
activates the right IFG, but not Broca’s area or the ATL. Frontiers in Psychology. 7:241.

Tivarus, M. E., Starling, S. J., Newport, E. L., & Langfitt, J. T. (2012). Homotopic language reorganization in the right hemisphere after early left hemisphere injury. Brain and language, 123(1), 1-10.

Tompkins, C. A., M. T. Lehman-Blake, A. Baumgaertner, and W. Fassbinder. 2001. Mechanisms of discourse comprehension impairment after right hemisphere brain damage: suppression in inferential ambiguity resolution. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 44(2), 400–15.

Tyler, L. K., Marslen-Wilson, W. D., Randall, B., Wright, P., Devereux, B. J., Zhuang, J., ... & Stamatakis, E. A. (2011). Left inferior frontal cortex and syntax: function, structure and behaviour in patients with left hemisphere damage. Brain134(2), 415-431.


Zempleni, M. Z., Renken, R., Hoeks, J. C., Hoogduin, J. M., & Stowe, L. A. (2007). Semantic ambiguity processing in sentence context: Evidence from event-related fMRI. Neuroimage34(3), 1270-1279.